|
Post by nicolegreenbaum on Apr 11, 2016 6:28:22 GMT
The bystander effect occurs when the presence of others discourages an onlooker from intervening in an emergency situation. Social psychologists Bibb Latané and John Darley popularized the concept following the infamous 1964 Kitty Genovese murder in New York City. Not one of the 38 bystanders of the murder stepped in to assist the victim or call the police. Latané and Darley attributed the bystander effect to:
- the perceived diffusion of responsibility (onlookers are more likely to intervene if there are few or no other witnesses)
- social influence (individuals in a group monitor the behavior of those around them to determine how to act)
- ambiguity (if others are present any one onlooker can assume that one of the other onlookers already took action)
In Genovese's case, the onlookers concluded from their neighbors' inaction that their own personal help was not needed. Often times, bystanders who do not intervene have rational fears: physical harm, public embarrassment, involvement with police procedures, and other unknown dangers. However, it is our moral obligation as humans to help those in need. Genovese’s killer, Winston Moseley, died in prison this week, bringing the case and its implications back into the spotlight. Even today, doing something to counteract the insidiousness of the bystander effect remains stubbornly difficult.
Here is a video that was actually filmed at UCLA and is a exceptional example of the bystander effect in everyday life.
Has there ever been a time in which bystander apathy effected you? If so, why didn't you feel the need to take action? How do social and cultural differences impact the way one perceives another person in danger?
|
|
paola
New Member
Posts: 34
|
Post by paola on Apr 11, 2016 21:52:39 GMT
Because the “bystander” effect is such a prevalent problem today, I have many different things to say about it.
In my opinion, the greatest factor that prevents someone from stepping in and helping a victim in a time of danger is fear - fear of what may happen to them if they were to step in and help, fear of getting involved with the police, etc. Another factor that prevents one from stepping in and helping is that we all have this common thought in our minds: “someone around me is bound to help (the victim), so I don’t need to”. The problem with such a thought is that we are all thinking it, resulting in absolutely nothing being done.
In the article you linked by The Guardian, it mentions the story about a homeless man named Hugo Tale-Yax who helped a woman who was being repeatedly knifed by an assailant. He eventually saved the woman, but was unfortunately greatly injured himself. As he lay there in his own pool of blood, nobody cared to assist him in any way. People walked by and stared. Others recorded the incident – something that happens A LOT in today's society. Recording an unjust event (and posting it on social media) is great for raising awareness, but it’s also a first-hand example of the bystander effect: the recorder is not helping the victim. Rather than recording the event, the recorder should step in and help, in my opinion.
Right now, at rest, we would think that if we ever witnessed an unjust act, we’d surely help the victim with no hesitation. This calculated decision is being made with reason because we are calm and perhaps not actually experiencing the event. In the event of the actual happening, one’s anxiety and fear (our emotions) inevitably come into play and push our reason out of the window. We suddenly let the uncertainty of what would happen to us if we were to step in haunt us. This fear and uncertainty is preventing us from making a good decision (to help the victim).
One way to solve this problem is to encourage the bystander to (if the incident allows such time) think carefully before acting, but to definitely ACT. If stress and anxiety prevents us from helping the victim, perhaps taking a moment to formulate a rational decision to help the victim would help. We must remind ourselves that, regardless of the danger it may put us in, the right thing to do is to help the victim.
|
|
|
Post by nicolegreenbaum on Apr 12, 2016 4:08:43 GMT
Thanks for your very informed response, Paola. I also found the story about Hugo Tale-Yax to be intriguing. It goes to show that even when one overcomes the bystander effect, there is not always a reward for one's bravery. This relates to another factor which contributes to the bystander effect: fear of physical harm. Tale-Yax was courageous enough to sacrifice himself for the safety of the woman, but in doing so he lost his own life. This brings me to the question: to what degree can we be expected to put our lives at risk in order to help save the life of someone else?
|
|
|
Post by elipshutz on Apr 12, 2016 4:35:12 GMT
The bystander apathy I have never fully experienced when it comes to emergency situations. However, I have witnessed it on a small scale. The most I am aware of it is at school during school projects. I feel during group projects the work is never equally distributed. Instead, people tend to rely on other group members to get the grade that they desire. Normally the majority of the work ends up being given to the student who cares the most about the grade, or who knows the most about the subject. People less interested tend to slack off and and let others take over for them. Although group projects are very different than someone someone reporting a crime, or stopping a murder, it is still the same idea of not doing an action because there are others involved. If the assignment is individual, people are required to complete the work or else it wont get done. Similarly, if there is only one witness they must intervene or else the crime will continue.
I think when it comes to more serious situations it is hard to tell how people will react unless they are actually experiencing the event. Personally, I would like to think I am the type of person that would intervene. However, if in the moment I feel unsafe, I cannot confidently say that I would. I think it really depends on the situation and how comfortable someone feels in the exact moment on whether or not they will respond. That being said, it is not a safe bet to assume that other people are responding. So if someone is comfortable they should act on it.
|
|
|
Post by Emily Leung on Apr 12, 2016 4:56:38 GMT
I definitely see the bystander effect playing out in my life. I am way less likely to offer help when I know that others are around to offer their help as well. Especially when someone is injured or something, I feel like I cannot offer adequate help for them so I pass off responsibility to other bystanders.
I think an interesting related experiment would be to test whether a person's environment (if they were raised in a city, suburb, etc.) changes their likeliness to offer up help.
|
|
|
Post by emmajessicalin on Apr 12, 2016 5:02:20 GMT
I've watched videos on other social experiments as well that have tested people's reactions to public rape, suicide attempts, or catcalling. It's definitely interesting to see in which situations people are most willing to step in and why this may be so. I think people would be more willing to help in situations in which they are least likely to be physically harmed.
While I can't currently think of a specific situation in which I've experienced bystander effect, I'm sure I have experienced it before. I agree that bystander effect can occur on a smaller scale in our daily lives.
|
|
|
Post by cliffordzhang on Apr 12, 2016 5:12:32 GMT
Your discussion reminded me of Good Samaritan Law which essentially provides legal protection to any licensed doctor or nurse if he/she offers assistance during a medical emergency. It was implemented specifically to combat the bystander effect because often first responders were hesitant to help because they could have been sued for wrongful death or injury. Here's the wikipedia article about it: en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Good_Samaritan_law#United_StatesAlso, I don't know if this is a real thing or not, but I think I remember reading something about how any licensed doctor or nurse MUST help if he/she is around a medical emergency and failure to do so can result in a variety of consequences. Anyone care to elaborate?
|
|
|
Post by nicolegreenbaum on Apr 12, 2016 6:28:23 GMT
In response to Clifford, I may be able to answer your question regarding whether or not a doctor MUST help in a medical emergency because my dad is an orthopedic surgeon. While I do not know if this applies to all doctors, my dad has told me that he would never turn away a patient that is in a life or death circumstance. He believes that as a doctor, it is his obligation to help those in need. In fact, during my parents' honeymoon he saved a woman's life. The woman was lying on the beach behind a mound of sand. Nearby, two men were throwing a javelin to each other, but one threw it too hard and it went over the mound of sand directly into the chest of the woman on the beach. The man who went to retrieve the javelin was stunned to see it had impaled the woman and immediately yelled asking for the help of a doctor. My dad was quick to join the scene and assist the woman until a helicopter rescue came to take her to the nearest hospital. The rescue team said that if my dad hadn't taken the actions he did to help the woman, she might have lost her life. Then, the hotel bought him a drink at the bar as a thanks. True story.
|
|
|
Post by dchang on Apr 13, 2016 4:03:07 GMT
Though I cannot completely recall a specific incident where bystander apathy has effected me, I'm pretty sure that it has happened to me before. In the specific case as shown through the video, I think there was a lack of action because people were scared to be physically harmed since majority of people would be more concerned with their own safety than the safety of others. True altruism in cases like these rarely exist, as humanity is generally more selfish than altruistic, especially towards a complete stranger. Also, I think that instead of saying that cultural/social differences change the way one perceives danger, I would say that these differences contribute to how a person may react in the bystander situation; for example, one may be more prone to help a person in need based on the values he or she were taught as a child.
|
|
|
Post by petekk on Apr 13, 2016 4:48:55 GMT
Paola's post made me think about the contribution of social media to the diffusion of responsibility. The example I am going to give does not apply necessarily to emergency situations but more generally to anything that requires action from participation in a political rally from adoption of a puppy. In my country (Turkey) when something outrageous happens such as a corruption scandal or a case police violence everyone post about it on their Facebook pages. I would guess that the number of people who actually go to protests are smaller then the people who post about it. In cases like this, sharing something on social media can be contributing to decrease our sense of responsibility. When we post something we actively include bystanders in the event and according to the results of the experiment we become less likely to act.
|
|
|
Post by jreitsma on Apr 13, 2016 5:18:44 GMT
Cultural and social differences do matter in terms of how one perceives danger. For example, If a fight broke out in a high-class society compared to a low-class society, the high-class society would probably be more likely to call 911 compared to the low-class society. This is due to low-class societies such as in Compton, California, where there is a high crime, drug use, and murder rate. People are afraid to call because, in the past criminals have found out about them calling the police, which in turn made them seek revenge on the ones who called, even if that means revenge by murder. So yes I do believe the way different societies perceive danger has an effect on the bystander empathy effect.
|
|
|
Post by eddiegonz on Apr 13, 2016 6:43:55 GMT
The bystander effect is a really interesting because it shows the dissociation of responsibility among the members of society when faced with a dilemma. In the Kitty case, the people were exposed to the murder of some stranger and since they saw that others were observers of the murder, the result was that no one bothered to do much about it. Some may say that the reason no one responded with care was not only because other people were watching, but also the fact that the people didn't have much of an emotional connection with the person and therefore they were less likely to act on the situation. Im more interested to know how a group of friends would react to witnessing a fellow friend experiencing a seizure. Would the bystander effect still apply and would the responsibility be dispersed among the friends, or would the variable be dependent on the kind of relationship held with the friend. In the situations presented in the experiments, is the fact that the person in danger is a stranger relevant in any way? Im sure the response to act diminishes as there are more people present in witnessing the situation, but my question is whether this still applies in a group where everyone knows each other and a friend or family member acts as the subject being affected. Its just interesting to see to what extent the bystander effect works and how culture, class, family and friends may be possible factors that may contribute to overall effect.
|
|
|
Post by nicolegreenbaum on Apr 14, 2016 3:53:52 GMT
Eddie, thanks for bringing up a very interesting point. I also wonder if having an emotional connection to the victim can change the Bystander Effect. The norm of social responsibility states that "people should help others who are in need of help and who are dependent on them for it". As suggested by the research, the more cohesive a group, the more likely the group will act in accordance to the social responsibility norm. To test this hypothesis, researchers used undergraduate students and divided them into four groups: a low cohesive group with two people, a low cohesive group with four people, a high cohesive group with two people and a high cohesive group with four people. The students in the high cohesive group were acquainted with each other by introducing themselves and discussing their likes and dislikes. The point of the experiment was to determine whether or not high cohesive groups were more willing to help a hurt victim than the low cohesive groups. The four member high cohesive groups were the quickest and most likely groups to respond to the victim who they believed to be hurt. The four member low cohesive groups were the slowest and least likely to respond to the victim.
|
|
|
Post by Giulia Sperandio on Apr 14, 2016 5:22:14 GMT
I associated the bystander effect with the other research about the power of social norms. Just as teenagers tend to use more drugs when they see it is common to use drugs I thought the same would happen to people who watched something: they would be prompted to act. But I then decided to see it through another perspective, as someone who masked the rest of the group instead of someone who reacted to the response of someone suffering. This is something we face in our every-day lives as we do not react to homeless people in the street, or to someone screaming with his/her boyfriend.
What really intrigued me after this research was the alarming fact that I could be a bystander. And even though I freaked out a little one question still remained: Is there a way to change the response in a bystander effect?
I found some factors that can help overcome the bystander effect according to Psychology Today:
Witnessing Helping Behavior
Being Observant
Being Skilled and Knowledgeable
Guilt
Having a Personal Relationship or Empathizing with others (making eye contact, saying Good Morning etc)
Seeing Others as Deserving of Help
Feeling Good or Having Confidence
One of these specifically stood out for me: Witnessing Helping Behavior, and that's where I link the other research. The same marketing campaigns that could help reduce drug consumption, could raise awareness to the Bystander Effect and prompt people to action.
|
|